Contribution of Dogs to White-Tailed Deer Hunting Success

Research Article

Contribution of Dogs to White-Tailed

Deer Hunting Success

CHRISTOPHER GODWIN,

1 Environmental and Life Sciences Graduate Program, Trent University, 1600 West Bank Drive, Peterborough, ON,

Canada K9J 7B8

JAMES A. SCHAEFER,

2 Biology Department, Trent University, 1600 West Bank Drive, Peterborough, ON, Canada K9J 7B8

BRENT R. PATTERSON,

Wildlife Research and Development Section, Ontario Ministry of Natural Resources, 2140 East Bank Drive,

Peterborough, ON, Canada K9J 7B8

BRUCE A. POND,

Wildlife Research and Development Section, Ontario Ministry of Natural Resources, 2140 East Bank Drive, Peterborough, ON,

Canada K9J 7B8

ABSTRACT

Dogs (Canis familiaris) are used in hunting white-tailed deer (Odocoileus virginianus) in

10 North American jurisdictions. Although the practice is longstanding and controversial, the effects of

dogs on the outcome of the hunt have rarely been studied. We evaluated the influence of dogs on recreational

hunting of white-tailed deer based on long-term data from southeastern Ontario, Canada. Over 25 years,

annual surveys of hunters were used to collect data on hunting effort and deer harvest from approximately

85 camps, roughly half of which had dogs. We investigated the relationship between harvest and 3 treatments

(i.e., 0 dogs, 1 dog, and

2 dogs in camp), interactions with weather and deer density, and effects of

neighboring camps. Dogs enhanced hunter success. We found no difference in deer encounter rates but, per

unit effort, camps with

2 dogs harvested 0.013 (26%) more deer per hunter-day, missed 0.010 (23%) more

deer per hunter-day, and wounded 0.002 (40%) more deer per hunter-day than camps without dogs.

Conversely, camps without dogs saw, without shooting at, 0.033 (23%) more deer per hunter-day than

camps with

2 dogs. These results are consistent with the idea that hunters with dogs are less selective.

Hunters with dogs harvested more fawns per unit effort, but we found no difference in the harvest rate of

older female deer. More precipitation, greater wind speed, lower temperatures and greater deer density

improved harvest success but had no differential effect among dog treatments. Hunter success at camps with



2 dogs was less when neighboring camps also had 2 dogs. Because antlerless deer quotas are the principal

means to control populations, increasing use of hunting dogs is unlikely to have substantial effects in

managing overabundant deer.

 2012 The Wildlife Society.

KEY WORDS

dogs, harvest, hunting, neighborhood effects, Odocoileus virginianus, weather, white-tailed deer,

wounding loss.

Overabundant populations of white-tailed deer (

Odocoileus

virginianus

) have major ecological and economic effects. In

North America, the annual cost of vehicle collisions and

damage to crops and forests is in the billions of dollars

(Conover 1997). At high density, deer can limit plant regeneration

and alter forest ecosystems (Campbell et al. 2004,

Coˆte´ et al. 2004). In many cases, sport hunting represents the

most effective and cost-efficient tool for controlling deer

abundance (Giles and Findlay 2004).

Dogs (

Canis familiaris) are permitted in white-tailed deer

hunting in 10 jurisdictions in North America, including

Ontario. The practice is longstanding but increasingly an

issue with the public and has generated continual debate

on the ethics of dog use, treatment of dogs, and trespass on

private lands (Lawrence 1993, Rabb 2010, Hansen 2011).

Although the subject of many popular articles, relationships

among dogs, hunter success, and deer biology have been the

focus of few studies. Some suggest little or no physiological

or demographic effects on deer (Progulske and Baskett 1957,

Marchinton et al. 1970, Gavitt et al. 1974, Gipson and

Sealander 1977); others have noted deer injuries and mortality

(Corbett et al. 1971, Nichols and Whitehead 1978) and

elevated stress hormones in deer populations hunted with

dogs (Bateson and Bradshaw 1997). Novak et al. (1991)

reported hunters with dogs were less selective and more

successful than still hunters. Hunting success, however,

may be influenced by many factors such as weather (Fobes

1945, Hansen et al. 1986), tactics of hunters in neighboring

areas (Milner-Gulland et al. 2004), and their interactions

(Perry and Giles 1971).

We used long-term observations from southeastern

Ontario to investigate the effects of hunting dogs on

white-tailed deer hunter success. Over 25 years, annual

data were collected on hunter effort and deer harvest from

Received: 24 January 2012; Accepted: 30 July 2012

1

Present address: Ontario Ministry of Agriculture, Food and Rural

Affairs, 581 Huron Street, Stratford, ON, Canada N5A 5T8.

2

E-mail: jschaefer@trentu.ca

The Journal of Wildlife Management; DOI: 10.1002/jwmg.474

Godwin et al.

Dogs and Deer Hunting Success 1

approximately 85 camps, roughly half of which had dogs. We

tested whether dogs affected the likelihood of harvesting,

wounding, missing and encountering deer, as well as age–sex

composition of the harvest. We also explored the interactions

of dogs with weather and deer density. Finally, because dogs

may influence the outcome of other hunters, we investigated

the potential spill-over effect of neighboring camps.

STUDY AREA

The Canonto Study Area (CSA; Fig. 1) covered 230 km

2

in southeastern Ontario (45

8080N, 768500W) along the

Canadian Shield; 88% of the CSA was Crown land.

Elevation ranged from 195 m to 396 m above sea level.

The landcover consisted mainly of hardwood forest with

some coniferous patches, interspersed with numerous water

bodies (Fryxell et al. 1991, Godwin 2010). Vehicle access was

primarily via a main road running through the center of the

CSA, a hydroelectric transmission line, and secondary roads

in the northern and southern limits (Fig. 1). The roads were

used by the public and provided access to permanent hunting

camps.

The firearms season for white-tailed deer was in November.

Before 1985, the season duration was 1-week and 2 weeks

thereafter. Party hunting—where hunters were allowed to

share game seals—was not allowed for antlerless deer in

1981–1991, although it likely remained common practice

in years when not permitted. The traditional method of

hunting deer with dogs was to place hunters on watch,

usually along roads or trails, while dogs and their handlers

moved to the opposite end of the area. The dogs were then

either released or kept on leash while handlers progressed

through the forest. Once dogs and handlers reached the line

of hunters on watch, dogs were collected. Most dogs let offleash

carried collars with return addresses, some with radio

transmitters, to prevent them from being lost.

The majority of hunting in our study area was from permanent

hunting camps (annual average

¼ 76 camps; range:

62–80). The permanent camps were relatively uniform in

their distribution, typically 1 km from the nearest neighbor

(Fig. 1). Each had a traditional hunting area with little

overlap among camps. In addition, on average 16 tent and

casual hunt camps (range: 12–19) occurred in the area in

1980–1986, which declined to an average of 7 such camps in

1988–2004 (range: 3–13). Not all camps hunted every year.

METHODS

Data Collection

Each year, 1980–2004, harvest information was collected

from an average of 85 hunting camps (range: 71–99).

Of those camps, approximately 40% had 0 dogs, 12% had

1 dog, and 48% had

2 dogs (max. 9 dogs). Data were

gathered, 1980–1986, from check station interviews and

thereafter from questionnaires mailed to the lead member

of each camp. Hunters were also individually canvassed by

Ministry of Natural Resources personnel each year. The

interviews and questionnaires requested information on

hunting activities for each day of the hunt: numbers of

hunters, dogs, antlerless permits, full-day (

4 hr) hunters,

half-day (

<4 hr) hunters, bucks killed, does killed, and deer

seen, missed, and wounded. In 1987, no information on the

number of dogs in camp was collected; we therefore excluded

1987 from analysis. We calculated annual hunter effort per

camp by summing full hunter-days (1.0) and half hunterdays

(0.5) across the season. We based effort for antlerless

deer on the number of permit-days per season, which we

estimated as the number of permits multiplied by the number

of days a camp operated. Despite the possibility of overestimation

of effort bias due to early filling of permits, and

underestimation due to party hunting, we believe that this

represented a better indication of effort than hunter-days for

the antlerless deer harvest. Hunters were asked to fill out the

forms daily during the hunt. The overall return rate of the

mailed surveys was 83.8%, an excellent response rate for mail

survey, indicating minimal non-response bias (Filion 1978).

Data Analysis

During 1980–1986, hunters were asked at check stations to

indicate their daily dog usage (as full day, half-day, or not

used). From this, we computed dog use over the hunting

season each year. On average, camps with 1 dog used it 58%

of the time spent hunting each season; camps with

2 dogs

used them 87–100% of hunting time (Godwin 2010).

Therefore, we identified 3 treatments: 0 dogs, 1 dog, and



2 dogs per camp. The clear relationship between number of

dogs in camp and dog use enabled us to use mail survey

results (1988–2004), which reported only the number of dogs

in camp during the hunt.

We identified 5 response variables, each computed annually

based on the total number of deer divided by total hunter

Figure 1.

The Canonto Study Area (CSA) in southeastern Ontario,

Canada. Roads (gray lines) and hunting camps (pentagons) are depicted.

2 The Journal of Wildlife Management

effort: kill per unit effort, number of deer harvested; seen per

unit effort, number of deer seen (but not shot at); missed per

unit effort, number of deer shot at and missed; wounded

per unit effort, number of deer wounded but not recovered;

and encountered per unit effort, the sum of kill, seen, missed,

and wounded per unit effort. All treatments followed a

normal distribution with no obvious outliers after visual

inspection and they passed Levene’s test for homogeneity

of variances (

P > 0.05) unless otherwise noted. We used

Statistica version 7 (Statsoft Inc., Tulsa, OK) in our analyses.

Kill per unit effort is a common metric for assessing harvest

success, but can be confounded if harvest success varies

systematically with effort (Lancia et al. 1996, Van Deelen

and Etter 2003, Giles and Findlay 2004). Indeed, we found

an accelerating relationship between total harvest (

K) and

effort (

E) where K ¼ 0.014  E1.182 (95% CI of the exponent:

1.005, 1.359). Moreover, effort varied among treatments

(Fig. 2), a discrepancy driven more by the number of

hunters per camp (

r ¼ 0.689) than the number of days

hunted (

r ¼ 0.279). Any differences in hunter success

among treatments, therefore, may have been confounded

by the number of hunters per camp (Fig. 2). To test for

this possibility, we controlled for camp size. We matched

camps into equal numbers of hunters (2–18 hunters per

camp) and calculated the mean difference in kill per unit

effort (and 95% CI) between treatments each year for each

matched group, under the null hypothesis of 0 difference.

Each year, on average, we were able to include 54, 30, and 29

camps in contrasts between

2 dogs and 0 dogs, 2 dogs and

1 dog, and 0 dogs and 1 dog, respectively. Some camps were

used in

1 contrast. We repeated this matching procedure

for the other response variables (seen, missed, wounded,

and encountered per unit effort) to investigate further

the potentially confounding influence of effort on harvest

success.

We used a mixed model analysis of variance (ANOVA) to

investigate the effect of dog use on dependent variables (kill,

missed, and encountered per unit effort) that satisfied the

assumptions for a parametric test. Treatment served as the

fixed variable and year as a random variable. We applied

Tukey’s Honestly Significant Difference (HSD) for post

hoc comparisons. Seen per unit effort and wounded

per unit effort failed Levene’s test, so we performed a

non-parametric Friedman 2-way ANOVA. We used the

Mann–Whitney

U-test with a Bonferroni-corrected a of

0.017 (i.e., 0.05/3 tests) as post hoc tests.

We investigated hunter success on the basis of 4 age–sex

classes of deer: adult bucks (

>1-yr-old males), adult does

(

>1-yr-old females), antlerless deer (female deer and male

fawns), and fawns. We computed each metric as the total

number killed per hunter- (or permit-) day each hunting

season. For these 4 response variables, we used the same

approach—a mixed-model ANOVA (for adult bucks

and adult does) and a Friedman 2-way ANOVA test (for

antlerless deer, which failed the Levene’s test).

We obtained observations from the closest meteorological

station with complete data in 1980–2004, Ottawa

International Airport approximately 100 km northeast of

the CSA (Environment Canada, unpublished data). We

used daily meteorological variables as predictors: maximum

temperature, minimum temperature, mean temperature, total

snowfall, total rainfall, total precipitation, snow on the

ground, and speed of maximum wind gust. Although winds

appear to have little effect on deer activity (Webb et al. 2010),

given the potential impact of strong winds on the ability

of hunters to detect deer, we predicted that the number

of days with strong winds (i.e.,

>31 km/hr) would be negatively

correlated with hunting success. We summed the

numbers of days per hunting season with wind gusts

(

>31 km/hr), snowfall, rainfall, snow on the ground, and

total precipitation per season. We calculated the average

maximum, minimum, and mean temperatures during each

season for each year.

We used principal component analysis (PCA) to collapse 8

weather variables, which exhibited substantial correlations,

into fewer synthetic variables. Based on the proportion of

variance explained (86%), we retained the first 3 PCA axes

(Table 1). The first component (PC1) consisted mainly of

average maximum, minimum, and mean temperature at the

negative end, and total precipitation and days with rainfall at

the positive end. The second component (PC2) consisted

mainly of days with gusts

>31 km/hr and days with snowfall

with a strong negative loading. The third component was

reflective of the number of days with snow on the ground.

Since days with snow on the ground was the only heavily

Figure 2.

Annual harvest of white-tailed deer in relation to annual hunting

effort, classified by number of dogs in camp, Canonto Study Area, Ontario,

1980–2004.

Table 1.

Eigenvectors and percent variance explained for 8 weather variables

on the first 3 axes (PC1, PC2, and PC3) from principal component analysis.

Data from Ottawa International Airport, 1980–2004.

Variable PC1 PC2 PC3

Percent variance explained 51.58 22.29 12.07

Days with snow on the ground 0.320 0.084 0.689

Days with snowfall 0.176

0.624 0.331

Days of rainfall 0.412 0.037 0.385

Total precipitation 0.353

0.438 0.121

Average max. temperature

0.428 0.204 0.280

Average min. temperature

0.410 0.259 0.289

Average mean temperature

0.441 0.245 0.299

Days with wind gusts

>31 km/hr 0.162 0.491 0.042

Godwin et al.

Dogs and Deer Hunting Success 3

weighted parameter, it was used in place of the third principal

component. We performed an analysis of covariance

(ANCOVA) with PC1, PC2, and days with snow on the

ground as covariates, kill per unit effort as the dependent

variable, and 3 dog treatments as predictor variables. To

test for interactions, we tested for heterogeneity of slopes

among treatments for the 3 covariates (Quinn and Keough

2002).

We followed Giles and Findlay (2004) by computing an

index of deer density from the residuals of the log–log plot of

number of deer encountered and hunter effort. We conducted

an ANCOVA with deer density as a covariate, kill per

unit effort as the dependent variable, and 3 dog treatments as

predictor variables. We tested for heterogeneity of slopes

among treatments as indicative of an interaction between

deer density and dog use.

We investigated the potential effect of neighboring camps

by assessing harvest success in relation to the number and

type of camps in the vicinity. Because we expected hunters to

exert a greater effect in close proximity, for each camp we

weighted linearly and negatively each adjacent camp by its

distance,2 km, to the focal camp (Fig. 1). For each year, we

fitted a regression between kill per unit effort and summed,

weighted number of neighboring camps, and then calculated

the mean slope and 95% confidence intervals across years.

We completed this separately for each treatment. We tested

for deviation from the null hypothesis of 0 slope. Because the

treatment effect was most pronounced between 0 dogs and



2 dogs (see Results Section), we focused on these 2 groups.

RESULTS

When we controlled for the number of hunters per camp, we

found results consistent with other analyses. The mean

difference in kill per unit effort and wounded per unit effort

between 0 dogs and

2 dogs was significantly greater than 0,

indicating greater harvest success and wounding rate of the



2 dog treatment (Table 2). In contrast, the mean differences

in seen per unit effort, missed per unit effort, and

encountered per unit effort between treatments were not

significantly different than 0 (Table 2). These results largely

corroborated those from the ANOVA where camp size was

not controlled for; of 15 contrasts, 11 were consistent with

the results from the ANOVA. Moreover, in no instance did a

monotonic relationship appear to exist between the magnitude

of the contrast and camp size (Godwin 2010). These

outcomes discount the potential confounding effect of camp

size on our results.

Based on the mixed modelANOVA, dogs had a substantial

effect on the outcome of the hunt. Encounter rates with deer

(encountered per unit effort) did not differ with respect to

dogs, but kill per unit effort varied significantly between

treatments (Table 3); hunters in camps with

2 dogs killed

0.013 (26%) more deer per hunter-day compared to those

with 0 dogs. Camps with

2 dogs also missed 0.010 (23%)

more deer per hunter-day and wounded 0.002 (40%) more

deer per hunter-day than camps without dogs. In contrast,

camps without dogs saw, without shooting, 0.033 (23%)

more deer per hunter-day than camps with

2 dogs

(Table 3).

The number of adult bucks killed per hunter-day differed

among treatments (Table 4). Hunters using

2 dogs killed

17% more adult bucks and 54% more fawns per hunter-day

compared to the 0 dog treatment, but we found no significant

treatment effect on the harvest success of adult does or

antlerless deer (Table 4).

Weather influenced hunting outcomes. We found a positive

relationship between kill per unit effort and PC1, indicating

greater success with less than average maximum,

Table 2.

Mean differences in white-tailed deer hunting success between dog treatments, Canonto Study Area, Ontario, 1980–2004, after controlling for

number of hunters per camp. Numbers in parentheses represent 95% confidence limits;

n represents number of camp sizes.

Dependent variable

Contrasts



2 dogs minus 0 dogs 2 dogs minus 1 dog 0 dogs minus 1 dog

Deer killed per unit effort 0.030 (0.017, 0.042)

; n ¼ 17 0.032 (0.030, 0.093); n ¼ 13 0.008 (0.029, 0.012); n ¼ 13

Deer seen per unit effort 0.108 (

0.146, 0.362); n ¼ 18 0.085 (0.133, 0.303); n ¼ 12 0.022 (0.068, 0.023); n ¼ 12

Deer missed per unit effort 0.003 (

0.011, 0.017); n ¼ 18 0.010 (0.002, 0.022); n ¼ 12 0.005 (0.009, 0.020); n ¼ 12

Deer wounded per unit effort 0.003 (0.001, 0.005)

; n ¼ 18 0.001 (0.005, 0.004); n ¼ 12 0.002 (0.006, 0.002); n ¼ 12

Deer encountered per unit effort 0.131 (

0.110, 0.373); n ¼ 18 0.126 (0.150, 0.403); n ¼ 12 0.027 (0.095, 0.041); n ¼ 12



P < 0.05.

Table 3.

Effects of hunting dogs on hunting success for white-tailed deer, Canonto Study Area, Ontario, 1980–2004. Superscripts (A, B) indicate no significant

difference between treatment means in post hoc tests.

Dependent variable Statistic

a df P

Treatment

0 dogs 1 dog

2 dogs

Deer killed per unit effort

F ¼ 11.86 2, 23 <0.001 0.051A 0.052A 0.064

Deer seen per unit effort

x2 ¼ 8.083 2 0.018 0.176A 0.164AB 0.143B

Deer missed per unit effort

F ¼ 5.34 2, 23 0.012 0.044A 0.046A 0.054

Deer wounded per unit effort

x2 ¼ 7.00 2 0.030 0.005A 0.006AB 0.007B

Deer encountered per unit effort

F ¼ 0.28 2, 23 0.758 0.275 0.267 0.267

a

F from mixed model analysis of variance (ANOVA); x2 from Friedman 2-way ANOVA.

4 The Journal of Wildlife Management

minimum, and mean temperatures; more precipitation;

and more days with rainfall (

r2 ¼ 0.16, P < 0.001), as

well as a negative relationship between kill per unit effort

and PC2, pointing to less success with fewer days with

gusts

>31 km/hr and fewer days with snowfall (r2 ¼ 0.06,

P

¼ 0.033). The number of days with snow on the ground

had no effect. We found no evidence of interactions. The

slopes of PC1 (

P ¼ 0.48), PC2 (P ¼ 0.97), and days with

snow on the ground (

P ¼ 0.54) with kill per unit effort were

homogenous among treatments.

A relatively larger deer population translated into greater

hunting success. We found a positive relationship between

deer density and kill per unit effort (

r2 ¼ 0.48, P < 0.001),

but no differential effect of density on kill per unit effort

among dog treatments. The slope of density and kill per unit

effort was homogenous among treatments (

P ¼ 0.80).

Hunting success was reduced in the vicinity of other camps,

but only when those camps had

2 dogs. The kill per unit

effort of focal camps with

2 dogs tended to decline with a

greater number of neighboring camps with

2 dogs

(Table 5). The slope of this relationship was significantly

less than 0. None of the other treatment combinations

showed a significant effect on the kill per unit effort from

neighboring camps (Table 5).

DISCUSSION

Dogs enhanced hunting success. In our study, with

2 dogs

in camp, hunters harvested more deer per unit effort, irrespective

of the number of hunters in camp, weather, deer

abundance, and number of neighboring camps, unless they

too used dogs. Improved hunting success from dogs has been

a popular notion, albeit the topic of few studies. In South

Carolina, deer were 2.4 times more likely to be killed in a

dog-hunted area compared to a non-dog hunted area (Novak

et al. 1991). Similarly, Scribner et al. (1985) found hunters

with dogs harvested more deer per unit effort compared to

non-dog, stand hunters. When camps switched from stand

hunting to using dogs, their success increased to the same

level as camps traditionally using dogs. These results are

consistent with the 26% increase in harvest rate (0.013

more deer per hunter-day) for camps with

2 dogs in the

CSA (Table 3).

Compared to dog hunters, stand hunters often have greater

opportunities to watch undisturbed deer until within range;

they can generally take their time to shoot (Novak et al. 1991,

Martinez et al. 2005). Indeed, CSA hunters with

2 dogs

fired at and missed significantly more deer, although encounter

rates with deer did not differ. This situation may also

account for the increase in wounding rates by dog hunters—a

surprising result given that dogs are often justified by their

utility in finding wounded deer (Jeanneney 1977). In our

study, any potential improvement in the recovery of wounded

deer with dogs apparently was insufficient in reducing

wounding rates. To the contrary, we estimate that hunting

with dogs (

2 dogs per camp) resulted in approximately 4

more deer wounded per season compared to a complete

absence of dogs in the CSA. Overall, these findings suggest

that hunters with dogs are less selective than those without

dogs. Indeed, stand hunters tend to display selectively for

older or heavier deer (Novak et al. 1991, Martinez et al.

2005). Dogs also affected the composition of the harvest.

Hunters with

2 dogs harvested significantly more adult

bucks and more fawns per hunter-day, but not antlerless

deer, compared to camps without dogs (Table 4).

Our study is not the first to question whether catch per unit

effort can serve as a reliable index of harvest success (Lancia

et al. 1996, Schmidt et al. 2005). In the CSA, because greater

effort was associated with dogs in camp as well as disproportionately

greater harvest (Fig. 2), our results were potentially

confounded. Camp size, the major contributor to effort,

varied systematically with dog treatments. Nevertheless, after

controlling for the number of hunters per camp, we found

broadly similar findings to those from the ANOVA;

i.e., significant treatment effects (especially

2 dogs) for

deer killed, wounded, and encountered. These are, arguably,

the most biologically relevant parameters in deer management

(Jeanneney 1977, Giles and Findlay 2004). We found a

discrepancy, nevertheless, in the magnitude of difference in

kill per unit effort between

2 dogs and 0 dogs, 0.030 deer

per hunter-day when camp size was controlled for (Table 2)

versus 0.013 deer per hunter-day from the ANOVA

(Table 3). This is likely attributable to differences in

the experimental unit when computing these averages. We

Table 4.

Effects of hunting dogs on the age–sex composition of harvest of white-tailed deer, Canonto Study Area, Ontario, 1980–2004. Superscripts (A, B)

indicate no significant difference between treatment means in post hoc tests.

Dependent variable Statistic

a df P

Treatment

0 dogs 1 dog

 2 dogs

Adult bucks killed per hunter-day

F ¼ 5.80 2, 23 0.009 0.024A 0.022A 0.028

Adult does killed per permit-day

F ¼ 0.784 2, 23 0.468 0.063 0.062 0.055

Antlerless deer killed per permit-day

x2 ¼ 0.067 2 0.967 0.088 0.092 0.092

Fawns killed per permit-day

F ¼ 9.44 2, 23 0.001 0.024A 0.031AB 0.037B

a

F computed from mixed model analysis of variance (ANOVA); x2 from Friedman 2-way ANOVA.

Table 5.

Mean slope of number of white-tailed deer killed per unit effort

versus number of neighboring camps 2 km, inversely weighted by distance,

Canonto Study Area, Ontario, 1980–2004. Focal camps and neighboring

camps were classified by treatment (

2 dogs or 0 dogs). Numbers in

parentheses represent upper and lower 95% confidence limits.

Focal camp

Neighboring camp(s)



2 dogs 0 dogs



2 dogs 0.025 (0.047, 0.002) 0.013 (0.052, 0.025)

0 dogs

0.008 (0.030, 0.013) 0.012 (0.011, 0.035)

Godwin et al.

Dogs and Deer Hunting Success 5

suggest the value based on the whole dataset (0.013) is more

accurate. Overall, these results imply that effort alone does

not account for the apparent differences among treatments.

We anticipated dogs would impart greater hunting success

when weather was unfavorable and deer were less abundant.

Lowry and McArthur (1978), for instance, suggested dogs

were more effective in pursuing deer when the ground

was covered with snow, as deep snow may impede deer.

Fobes (1945) also noted that white-tailed deer harvest was

weather-dependent (i.e., increased total harvest in warm, wet

conditions). Our results differed somewhat from expectation.

Although we uncovered a positive effect of cold and wet

conditions on hunter success, we also found no interaction

with dog treatments, even though scenting by dogs can

increase in warm and wet conditions (Styrotuck 1972).

Hansen et al. (1986) noted snowfall and high wind speeds

created unfavorable hunting conditions, and were correlated

negatively with harvest in Illinois. In contrast, snowfall and

wind had a positive relationship with kill per unit effort in

the CSA.

Not surprisingly, harvest success tended to increase with

deer density (Hansen et al. 1986). Although some have

suggested the effectiveness of dogs may be inversely density

dependent (Perry and Giles 1971), we found no interaction

of the impact of dogs with deer abundance. Hunter-deer

encounters are also the basis for tracking population trends

(Giles and Findlay 2004). In our study, dogs appeared to

have no differential effect in deer encounter rates (Table 3),

which lends confidence in this population index.

Camps with dogs diminished the hunting success of their

neighbors. Where hunting camps had

2 dogs, kill per unit

effort of camps within 2 km was reduced if they too used

dogs (Table 5). Foster et al. (1997) suggested deer hunting

was dependent on hunter densities. At low hunter densities,

hunters are too scattered to encourage deer movement,

whereas at high densities, hunter interference reduces the

per capita harvest. Spillover effects have been documented in

Scotland, too, where unhunted estates can act as sources,

allowing for migration of deer into heavily hunted estates

(Milner-Gulland et al. 2004). At a broader scale, however,

Giles and Findlay (2004) found little evidence of hunter

interference between wildlife management units in central

Ontario.

MANAGEMENT IMPLICATIONS

Hunting remains the principal tool for controlling whitetailed

deer populations and their adverse ecological and

economic effects. Our study shows that dogs affect the

outcome of the recreational hunt; 0.013 (26%) more deer

were harvested per hunter-day in camps with

2 dogs,

although dogs did not enhance the harvest rates of antlerless

deer nor adult does. Because antlerless deer quotas represent

the primary means to controlling populations, increasing the

use of hunting dogs is likely to have marginal effects in

managing overabundant deer. Camps with dogs in close

proximity (2 km) may negatively influence one another’s

success. Increasing the dispersion of hunters with dogs,

therefore, is likely to enhance hunter success and satisfaction.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank the numerous CSA hunters for their collaboration,

L.S. Duquette for statistical guidance, and 2 anonymous

reviewers for their ideas and suggestions. We are

grateful to P.C. Smith who provided insightful comments

on the manuscript and whose longstanding work in the

CSA (1958–2004) formed the foundation of this paper.

This project was supported by Ontario Ministry of Natural

Resources and a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research

Council of Canada Discovery Grant to J.A.S.

LITERATURE CITED

Bateson, P., and E. L. Bradshaw. 1997. Physiological effects of hunting red

deer (

Cervus elaphus). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series

B-Biological Sciences 264:1707–1714.

Campbell, T. A., B. R. Laseter, W. M. Ford, and K. V. Miller. 2004.

Feasibility of localized management to control white-tailed deer in forest

regeneration areas. Wildlife Society Bulletin 32:1124–1131.

Conover, M. R. 1997. Monetary and intangible valuation of deer in the

United States. Wildlife Society Bulletin 25:298–305.

Corbett, R. L., R. L. Marchinton, and C. E. Hill. 1971. Preliminary study of

the effects of dogs on radio-equipped deer in a mountainous habitat.

Proceedings of the Annual Conference of Southeastern Association of

Fish and Wildlife Agencies 25:69–77.

Coˆte´, S. D., T. P. Rooney, J.-P. Tremblay, C. Dussault, and D. M. Waller.

2004. Ecological impacts of deer overabundance. Annual Review of

Ecology Evolution and Systematics 35:113–147.

Filion, F. L. 1978. Increasing the effectiveness of mail surveys. Wildlife

Society Bulletin 6:135–141.

Fobes, C. B. 1945. Weather and the kill of white-tailed deer in Maine.

Journal of Wildlife Management 9:76–78.

Foster, J. R., J. L. Roseberry, and A. Woolf. 1997. Factors influencing

efficiency of white-tailed deer harvest in Illinois. Journal of Wildlife

Management 61:1091–1097.

Fryxell, J. M., D. J. T. Hussell, A. B. Lambert, and P. C. Smith. 1991. Time

lags and population fluctuations in white-tailed deer. Journal of Wildlife

Management 55:377–385.

Gavitt, J. D., R. L. Downing, and B. S. McGinnes. 1974. Effect of dogs on

deer reproduction in Virginia. Proceedings of the Annual Conference of

Southeastern Association of Fish and Wildlife Agencies 28:532–539.

Giles, B. G., and C. S. Findlay. 2004. Effectiveness of a selective harvest

system in regulating deer populations in Ontario. Journal of Wildlife

Management 68:266–277.

Gipson, P. S., and J. A. Sealander. 1977. Ecological relationship of whitetailed

deer and dogs in Arkansas. Pages 3–16

in Proceedings of the 1975

Predator Symposium. Montana Forest and Conservation Experiment

Station, Missoula, Montana, USA.

Godwin, C. 2010. The contribution of dogs to hunter success in white-tailed

deer (

Odocoileus virginianus) harvest in south-eastern Ontario. Thesis,

Trent University, Peterborough, Ontario, Canada.

Hansen, L. 2011. Extensive management. Pages 409–451

in D. G. Hewitt,

editor. Biology and management of white-tailed deer. CRC Press, Boca

Raton, Florida, USA.

Hansen, L. P., C. M. Nixon, and F. Loomis. 1986. Factors affecting daily

and annual harvest of white-tailed deer in Illinois. Wildlife Society

Bulletin 14:368–376.

Jeanneney, J. 1977. The use of leashed trailing dogs for tracking wounded

deer. Transactions of the Northeast Fish and Wildlife Conference 34:143–

151.

Lancia, R. A., J. W. Bishir, M. C. Conner, and C. S. Rosenberry. 1996. Use

of catch-effort to estimate population size. Wildlife Society Bulletin

24:731–737.

Lawrence, R. 1993. Hunting deer and moose with dogs. Animals’ Voice

Winter:5–8.

Lowry, D. A., and K. L. McArthur. 1978. Domestic dogs as predators on

deer. Wildlife Society Bulletin 6:38–39.

Marchinton, R. L., A. S. Johnson, J. R. Sweeney, and J. M. Sweeney. 1970.

Legal hunting of white-tailed deer with dogs: biology, sociology and

6 The Journal of Wildlife Management

management. Proceedings of the Annual Conference of Southeastern

Association of Fish and Wildlife Agencies 24:74–89.

Martinez, M., C. Rodriguez-Vigal, O. R. Jones, T. Coulson, and A. San

Miguel. 2005. Different hunting strategies select for different weights in

red deer. Biology Letters 1:353–356.

Milner-Gulland, E. J., T. Coulson, and T. H. Clutton-Brock. 2004. Sex

differences and data quality as determinants of income from hunting red

deer

Cervus elaphus. Wildlife Biology 10:187–201.

Nichols, R. G., and C. J. Whitehead, Jr. 1978. The effects of dog harassment

on relocated white-tailed deer. Proceedings of the Annual

Conference of Southeastern Association of Fish and Wildlife Agencies

32:195–201.

Novak, J. M., K. T. Scribner, W. D. Dupont, and M. H. Smith. 1991.

Catch-effort estimation of white-tailed deer population size. Journal of

Wildlife Management 55:31–38.

Perry, M. C., and R. H. Giles, Jr. 1971. Studies of deer-related dog activity

in Virginia. Proceedings of the Annual Conference South Eastern

Association of Game and Fish Commissions 24:64–73.

Progulske, D., and T. S. Baskett. 1957. Mobility of Missouri deer and

their harassment by dogs. Journal of Wildlife Management 22:184–

192.

Quinn, G. P., and M. J. Keough. 2002. Experimental design and data

analysis for biologists. Cambridge University Press, United Kingdom.

Rabb, J. 2010. The dog-hunting debate. North American Whitetail.

<

http://www.northamericanwhitetail.com/2010/09/22/deermanagement_

naw_debate_1009/2/

>. Accessed 8 Aug 2012.

Schmidt, J. I., J. M. V. Hoef, J. A. K. Maier, and R. T. Bowyer. 2005. Catch

per unit effort for moose: a new approach using Weibull regression.

Journal of Wildlife Management 69:1112–1124.

Scribner, K. T., M. C. Wooten, M. H. Smith, and P. E. Johns. 1985.

Demographic and genetic characteristics of white-tailed deer populations

subjected to still or dog hunting. Pages 197–212

in Game Harvest

Management. Caesar Kleberg Wildlife Research Institute, Kingsville,

Texas, USA.

Styrotuck, W. G. 1972. Scent and the scenting dog. Arner, Rome, New

York, USA.

Van Deelen, T. R., and D. R. Etter. 2003. Effort and the functional response

of deer hunters. Human Dimensions of Wildlife 8:97–108.

Webb, S. L., K. L. Gee, B. K. Strickland, S. Demarais, and R. W. DeYoung.

2010. Measuring fine-scale white-tailed deer movements and environmental

influences using GPS collars. International Journal of Ecology,

vol. 2010, Article ID 459610, 12 pages. DOI: 10.1155/2010/459610

<

http://www.hindawi.com/journals/ijeco/2010/459610/cta/>.

Associate Editor: David Euler.

Godwin et al.

 

Dogs and Deer Hunting Success 7